Getting to the Top of Mind: How Reminders Increase Saving

Last registered on July 27, 2016

Pre-Trial

Trial Information

General Information

Title
Getting to the Top of Mind: How Reminders Increase Saving
RCT ID
AEARCTR-0001350
Initial registration date
July 27, 2016

Initial registration date is when the trial was registered.

It corresponds to when the registration was submitted to the Registry to be reviewed for publication.

First published
July 27, 2016, 12:00 PM EDT

First published corresponds to when the trial was first made public on the Registry after being reviewed.

Locations

Region
Region

Primary Investigator

Affiliation
Northwestern University

Other Primary Investigator(s)

PI Affiliation
Harvard University
PI Affiliation
Harvard School of Public Health
PI Affiliation
Dartmouth College
PI Affiliation
Yale University

Additional Trial Information

Status
Completed
Start date
2006-02-01
End date
2008-12-31
Secondary IDs
Abstract
We provide evidence from fi eld experiments with three different banks, that reminder messages increase commitment attainment for clients who recently opened commitment savings accounts. Messages that mention both savings goals and financial incentives are particularly effective, while other content variations such as gain versus loss framing do not have significantly different effects. Nor do we find evidence that receiving additional late reminders has an additive effect. These empirical results do not map neatly into existing models, so we provide a simple model where limited attention to exceptional expenses can generate under-saving that is in turn mitigated by reminders.
External Link(s)

Registration Citation

Citation
Karlan, Dean et al. 2016. "Getting to the Top of Mind: How Reminders Increase Saving." AEA RCT Registry. July 27. https://doi.org/10.1257/rct.1350-1.0
Former Citation
Karlan, Dean et al. 2016. "Getting to the Top of Mind: How Reminders Increase Saving." AEA RCT Registry. July 27. https://www.socialscienceregistry.org/trials/1350/history/9772
Sponsors & Partners

There is information in this trial unavailable to the public. Use the button below to request access.

Request Information
Experimental Details

Interventions

Intervention(s)
We examine whether an initiative to promote savings can help poor individuals living in semi-urban areas of Peru save more and switch from informal savings to formal sector methods. The study is implemented by the Caja Ica, a bank designed to serve the needs of poor clients with microsavings and microcredit programs, with program support from Catholic Relief Services (CRS) and technical assistance from COPEME. The Caja Ica is offering a new commitment savings product called "Ahorro Programmado". Clients who choose to participate in this service commit to saving an amount of their choosing, amounting to at least 20 soles (US$6.50) per month for 6, 12, 18, or 24 months. As an incentive for meeting their savings commitment, clients receive a preferential interest rate of more than twice what the normal interest rate is for savings accounts. We are evaluating various product designs, beyond the already increased financial incentive, to see which are more effective at encouraging clients to complete their savings commitment. Each of the estimated 5,000 clients expected to enroll in the program is randomly assigned to receive one or more of the following:

(1) reminder letters before the due date of their payment,
(2) token gifts upon payment to bring forward the "benefit" of saving,
(3) positive or negative incentive messages on each deposit slip, or
(4) no services, serving as a comparison.

This study determines the commitment device that most effectively encourages clients to meet their savings goals. We also conduct similar trials in Bolivia and the Philippines and summarise all results in one study.
Intervention Start Date
2006-02-01
Intervention End Date
2008-12-31

Primary Outcomes

Primary Outcomes (end points)
Probability of meeting set savings goals
Primary Outcomes (explanation)

Secondary Outcomes

Secondary Outcomes (end points)
Secondary Outcomes (explanation)

Experimental Design

Experimental Design
The three experiments were implemented by three different banks in three different country “sites”: Bolivia, Peru, and the Philippines. The sample for each experiment is comprised of new commitment savings account holders. What is meant by “commitment savings account” differs in each of the three sites. After opening the account, the bank randomly assigned reminder treatments: reminder or not, and then content and timing within the reminder group. Banks did not mention or advertise reminders prior to random assignment, nor did they create random assignments for non-takers, and hence we conduct our analysis on account-openers only. In two of three sites – Bolivia and Philippines - the reminders were not even announced at account-opening; the bank just started sending them at no charge to clients. Each bank also had its personnel collect some “baseline” data prior to making the product offer. Products, marketing, and some reminder features varied across sites.

First Valley Bank (FVB), a for-profit bank operating in Western Mindanao, Philippines, worked with us to randomize reminders as part of the rollout of its new Gihandom (Dream) Savings product. Between April and August 2007, bank marketing employees conducted door-to-door marketing visits in rural and small urban areas and offered 10,056 individuals the opportunity to open a Gihandom account. As part of this marketing visit, the bank employee also conducted a brief five to ten minute survey. Bank staff used personal digital accessories (PDAs) for the baseline survey and random assignment to treatments. Of the 10,056 offers, 2,314 (23%) opened an account. Gihandom allows a client to set her own commitment amount (US$50 or above) and commitment end-date (from three months to two years after opening). Except in hardship cases, clients can then access funds only once both commitments - amount and date - have been met. Once the client opens the account with a minimum deposit of US$2.50, there is no fixed deposit schedule to fulfill. The client receives a savings lockbox and is encouraged at sign-up to make small deposits on a daily basis. When the client desires, the client goes to the bank to deposit the money in the lockbox (e.g., when it is full). Clients also can, and do, go to the bank and make normal deposit, without the lockbox. We do not have data on whether they client had the lockbox with them for the deposit. Among clients with a cell phone (66% of those who opened accounts), the bank randomly and independently assigned some clients to receive “regular” and/or “late” text message reminders to come to the bank to make a deposit each month. The bank sent the late reminder only if the client did not make any deposit in a given month. Reminders were further randomized to gain or loss frame language with respect to “making your dream come true”. A client assigned to receive both regular and late reminders got the same frame on all messages.

In Peru, the Government-owned bank Caja de Ica worked with us to randomize reminders as part of the rollout of its new product Plan Ahorro (“Saving Plan”). The bank marketed the product on television and radio in the Ica metropolitan area (urban and rural), and clients signed up over the course of several months. When opening an account, Plan Ahorro clients selected a commitment end-date (between 6 months and 12 months post-opening), a minimum commitment amount to deposit each month, and a goal (specific expenditure) label from 14 pre-established categories. Clients were required to make each planned deposit within ten days of each monthly due date in order to meet their commitment. Commitment compliance was rewarded with an annualized interest rate of 8% per annum rather than the normal 4% per annum. As at our other sites, the bank randomly assigned reminders to clients after they signed up for the product. The bank sent letters because low cell phone prevalence made text messages impractical. As in the Philippines, the bank did independent randomizations for regular and/or late reminders that were assigned to the same gain or loss frame. The bank sent regular reminders with a target client-receipt date seven days before the due date for that month's scheduled deposit. The bank also randomly assigned regular-reminder clients to have their reminders signed by either the bank, or the client herself, with her signature recorded at account-opening. As in the Philippines, the bank sent a late reminder only if the client was late (i.e., if they had not made a deposit three days after their scheduled deposit date). All late-reminder letters were signed by the bank. The bank implemented two additional treatments designed to increase the salience of the client's specific expenditure goal. One treatment randomly assigned some in the reminder group to get a letter that focused on their particular goal. Another treatment independently and randomly assigned the gift clients received upon opening the account: a jigsaw puzzle of their goal, a photo of their goal, or a pen. Those in the jigsaw puzzle group received a piece of the puzzle after each deposit.

Ecofuturo, a for-profit bank in Bolivia, worked with us to implement a text message reminder program for its established product Ecoaguinaldo. “Aguinaldo” is the year-end bonus, equal to one month's pay, that employers are required to pay salaried employees in Bolivia. Ecofuturo markets Ecoaguinaldo as a product designed to help its clients, many of whom are self-employed, save up all year for their own year-end payout. The product is marketed for three months between January and March on television and radio in urban areas of Bolivia close to Ecofuturo's branches. Clients who opened an account during January-March 2008 were brought into the study and eligible to make savings deposits until the December 2008 commitment end-date. At sign-up, clients chose a monthly minimum deposit amount (with a floor of US$1.41). Clients making all of their committed monthly deposits received a bonus interest rate of 6% for their first ten months following enrollment in the program (compared to a regular interest rate of 3%) as well as free life and accident insurance.10 Clients missing one deposit, or withdrawing money before the payout date, forfeited the higher interest rate and had their insurance policies canceled. As in the Philippines, clients with a cell phone were randomly assigned to receive text message reminders or not. In Bolivia the experimental design called for everyone assigned to get a reminder to get one every month, in advance of their scheduled deposit; i.e., all reminders were supposed to be “regular,” and unlike the other two sites the bank did not randomly assign variation in regular versus late reminders. But the bank deviated from this design in two ways. First, it did not start sending reminders until several months after account opening (which took place January-March 2008), beginning in May. Second, the bank switched from a “regular” to “late” reminder rule starting in August: from August-November, clients were only sent a reminder if they had not yet made a deposit that month. As a consequence most clients received fewer reminders than prescribed by the design. (We use the random assignments in our analysis, rather than actual treatment status, to avoid bias from the endogeneity of not yet having made a deposit.) Interestingly, we see that lower-frequency messaging does not seem to reduce the potency of reminders. This suggests that it is the initial reminders that matter (consistent with “tuning out” or habituation over time), although we lack the random variation required to test that hypothesis. Every reminder mentioned the savings goal, broadly speaking: “Aguinaldo,” the year-end bonus (note that this goal is somewhat more generic than at the Peru site, where many clients indicated a more specific future goal like school fees). Besides the boilerplate script content was randomized 2x2: (gain or loss frame) x (mention insurance incentive or not). The latter treatment parallels the Peru design in the sense that it produces some reminders that mention both a financial incentive and a savings goal (in this case, the “Aguinaldo” or year-end bonus).
Experimental Design Details
Randomization Method
Personal digital accessories (PDAs)
Randomization Unit
Individual
Was the treatment clustered?
No

Experiment Characteristics

Sample size: planned number of clusters
Peru: 2,775 clients
Philippines: 1,409 clients
Bolivia: 9,376 clients
Total: 13,560 clients
Sample size: planned number of observations
13,560 clients in total (combined across all countries)
Sample size (or number of clusters) by treatment arms
Treatment groups (reminders sent) - Peru:
1) only regular timing and positive content: 174 clients
2) only regular timing and negative content: 168 clients
3) regular and late timing and positive content: 1,082 clients
4) regular and late timing and negative content: 1,012 clients

Treatment groups (reminders sent) - Philippines:
1) only regular timing and positive content: 163 clients
2) only regular timing and negative content: 187 clients
3) regular and late timing and positive content: 397 clients
4) regular and late timing and negative content: 410 clients

Treatment groups (reminders sent) - Bolivia:
1) regular and late timing and positive content: 2,350 clients
2) regular and late timing and negative content: 2,350 clients
Minimum detectable effect size for main outcomes (accounting for sample design and clustering)
IRB

Institutional Review Boards (IRBs)

IRB Name
Study has received IRB approval. Details not available.
IRB Approval Date
Details not available
IRB Approval Number
Details not available

Post-Trial

Post Trial Information

Study Withdrawal

There is information in this trial unavailable to the public. Use the button below to request access.

Request Information

Intervention

Is the intervention completed?
Yes
Intervention Completion Date
December 31, 2008, 12:00 +00:00
Data Collection Complete
Yes
Data Collection Completion Date
December 31, 2008, 12:00 +00:00
Final Sample Size: Number of Clusters (Unit of Randomization)
Peru: 2,775 clients
Philippines: 1,409 clients
Bolivia: 9,376 clients
Total: 13,560 clients
Was attrition correlated with treatment status?
Final Sample Size: Total Number of Observations
13,560 clients in total (combined across all countries)
Final Sample Size (or Number of Clusters) by Treatment Arms
Treatment groups (reminders sent) - Peru: 1) only regular timing and positive content: 174 clients 2) only regular timing and negative content: 168 clients 3) regular and late timing and positive content: 1,082 clients 4) regular and late timing and negative content: 1,012 clients Treatment groups (reminders sent) - Philippines: 1) only regular timing and positive content: 163 clients 2) only regular timing and negative content: 187 clients 3) regular and late timing and positive content: 397 clients 4) regular and late timing and negative content: 410 clients Treatment groups (reminders sent) - Bolivia: 1) regular and late timing and positive content: 2,350 clients 2) regular and late timing and negative content: 2,350 clients
Data Publication

Data Publication

Is public data available?
No

Program Files

Program Files
Reports, Papers & Other Materials

Relevant Paper(s)

Abstract
We provide evidence from fi eld experiments with three different banks, that reminder messages increase commitment attainment for clients who recently opened commitment savings accounts. Messages that mention both savings goals and financial incentives are particularly effective, while other content variations such as gain versus loss framing do not have significantly different effects. Nor do we find evidence that receiving additional late reminders has an additive effect. These empirical results do not map neatly into existing models, so we provide a simple model where limited attention to exceptional expenses can generate under-saving that is in turn mitigated by reminders.
Citation
Karlan, Dean, Margaret McConnell, Sendhil Mullainathan, Jonathan Zinman. "Getting to the Top of Mind: How Reminders Increase Saving." Working Paper, October 2014.

Reports & Other Materials