Voters' Demand for Price Caps

Last registered on March 11, 2023

Pre-Trial

Trial Information

General Information

Title
Voters' Demand for Price Caps
RCT ID
AEARCTR-0010626
Initial registration date
December 12, 2022

Initial registration date is when the trial was registered.

It corresponds to when the registration was submitted to the Registry to be reviewed for publication.

First published
January 03, 2023, 11:52 AM EST

First published corresponds to when the trial was first made public on the Registry after being reviewed.

Last updated
March 11, 2023, 2:11 PM EST

Last updated is the most recent time when changes to the trial's registration were published.

Locations

Region

Primary Investigator

Affiliation
University of Rosario

Other Primary Investigator(s)

PI Affiliation
Universidad del Rosario
PI Affiliation
Universidad del Rosario

Additional Trial Information

Status
On going
Start date
2022-10-27
End date
2023-05-01
Secondary IDs
Prior work
This trial does not extend or rely on any prior RCTs.
Abstract
We plan to conduct a laboratory experiment, combined with a survey, to study citizens’ support for price caps in competitive markets. We are interested in understanding how such support is affected by external information about the effects of price caps, direct exposure to them, and attitudes toward market regulation.
External Link(s)

Registration Citation

Citation
Aycinena, Diego, Stanislao Maldonado and Santiago Sautua. 2023. "Voters' Demand for Price Caps." AEA RCT Registry. March 11. https://doi.org/10.1257/rct.10626-1.1
Sponsors & Partners

There is information in this trial unavailable to the public. Use the button below to request access.

Request Information
Experimental Details

Interventions

Intervention(s)
We study citizens' support for price caps in the laboratory. Participants in our lab experiments will be exposed to alternative market regimes and information regarding their effects, such that we can understand citizens' support for price caps. We will examine several factors that could drive such support. First, we will test whether limited exposure to price caps increases the support for this policy. This may be the case if citizens fail to anticipate the negative effects of price caps on market outcomes. Second, because, in practice, citizens delegate the choice of policies to politicians, we will test whether citizens' lack of agency increases their support for price caps. Third, we will test whether providing information about the market consequences of price caps reduces citizens’ support. Finally, we shall also study the role of citizens' political attitudes and their attitudes toward market regulation.
Intervention Start Date
2022-11-16
Intervention End Date
2023-02-28

Primary Outcomes

Primary Outcomes (end points)
The primary outcomes of the experiment are the regime choices and beliefs about market outcomes.
Primary Outcomes (explanation)

Secondary Outcomes

Secondary Outcomes (end points)
Secondary Outcomes (explanation)

Experimental Design

Experimental Design
Subjects are randomly assigned into one of three treatment arms: Free Market First-Experience (FM-E), Free Market First-Information (FM-I), and Price Cap First-Experience (PC-E). To facilitate logistics, all subjects participating in a given experimental session are assigned to the same treatment arm. In the FM-E and FM-I treatments, both the practice trials and the first block of the Market Task feature a free market. By contrast, in the PC-E treatment, both the practice trials and the first block of the Market Task feature a price cap of 7 ECU.

Once the first block finishes, subjects in the FM-E treatment are informed that a price cap of 7 ECU will be imposed in the second block, whereas subjects in the PC-E treatment are informed that the initially imposed price cap of 7 ECU will be removed in the second block. Subjects in the FM-I treatment do not trade during the second block.

In all three treatments, before the second block begins, subjects choose the regime to be implemented in the third block, namely a free market or a market with a 7,000 price cap. Specifically, one buyer in each group is randomly assigned to choose the regime for the third block in his or her group. We refer to this person as the decisive chooser. Each of the three remaining buyers in the group also indicates which regime he or she would like to implement in the third block, but this choice is hypothetical. We refer to these three people as the hypothetical choosers. Importantly, each subject learns whether he or she is a decisive or hypothetical chooser before choosing the regime for the third block. At this point, subjects do not learn the choices of others.

Before the third block begins, subjects choose the regime to be implemented in the fourth block. In each group, the buyer who was previously assigned to be decisive remains decisive, while the others remain as hypothetical choosers. Buyers are reminded of their role before making a choice. At this point, subjects do not learn the choices of others.

Throughout the session, subjects report a series of beliefs. The details are provided in the Pre-Analysis Plan.
Experimental Design Details
Randomization Method
Randomization was done by a computer based on an index of political beliefs.
Randomization Unit
Individual.
Was the treatment clustered?
Yes

Experiment Characteristics

Sample size: planned number of clusters
175 groups of 4 participants.
Sample size: planned number of observations
700 observations.
Sample size (or number of clusters) by treatment arms
700 participants.
Minimum detectable effect size for main outcomes (accounting for sample design and clustering)
Using a within-subjects design, we assume a small effect size of 0.2 standard deviations in the primary outcome following Cohen's suggestion. We also assume 80% power and a one-sided test for differences in paired means with a 5% significance level. To account for clustering, we assume a cluster size of 4 and an intra-cluster correlation of 0.5. We adjust for 3 comparisons using the Bonferroni approach. Under these assumptions, the minimum sample size is 240 subjects without accounting for clustering. Assuming a design effect of 2.5, the estimated sample size is 600 subjects. If we use a McNemar test instead for discrete outcomes, the estimated sample size is similar. In both cases, we assume an autocorrelation of 0.5. Performing exact power calculations for non-parametric tests is not feasible because we do not know the shape of the underlying distribution. Therefore, some scholars use simulations to approximate power, making some distributional assumptions. At this stage, we follow a rule of thumb proposed by Lehmann (2006), who suggests adding 15% more subjects to the sample size estimated under a parametric approach. Hence, the estimated sample size is 690 subjects. This sample size is similar to the one used by Dal Bo et al. (2018), the closest antecedent in the empirical literature. In their experiment, they used 768 subjects. Therefore, our power calculations suggest that a sample of close to 700 subjects will be enough for this study. A final piece of our power analysis is the sample requirement for studying the role of pivotality. Some of the proposed tests in this regard require the use of interactions. Power calculations for interactions are typically very demanding. Therefore, we consider this analysis as exploratory.
IRB

Institutional Review Boards (IRBs)

IRB Name
Universidad del Rosario
IRB Approval Date
2022-06-09
IRB Approval Number
DVO 005 625-CS391
Analysis Plan

There is information in this trial unavailable to the public. Use the button below to request access.

Request Information

Post-Trial

Post Trial Information

Study Withdrawal

There is information in this trial unavailable to the public. Use the button below to request access.

Request Information

Intervention

Is the intervention completed?
No
Data Collection Complete
Data Publication

Data Publication

Is public data available?
No

Program Files

Program Files
Reports, Papers & Other Materials

Relevant Paper(s)

Reports & Other Materials