Intervention (Hidden)
I employ a Germany-wide representative survey (Freundl et al., 2022) to conduct an information provision experiment based on the methodology proposed by Haaland et al. (2023). The intervention begins by eliciting participants' prior beliefs about the relative performance of different groups in a standardized test via the following questions:
A test regularly examines the mathematics performance of 4th grade elementary school students in Germany (TIMSS = Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study).
How do you think each of the following groups of children compare on the test?
i) … What do you estimate is the case for girls and boys? ii) … What do you estimate, how is that for children from different social backgrounds?
iii) … What do you estimate, how is that with children without and with a migration background?
Responses are recorded on an eleven-point Likert scale, with one group positioned on one end and the other group on the opposite end (e.g., "1 - girls are much better," "3 - girls are somewhat better," "6 - both equally good," "9 - boys are somewhat better," "11 - boys are much better"). To counteract ordering effects, the Likert scale is inverted for half of the participants. Additionally, participants indicate their level of confidence in the estimated response on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from "1 - very unconfident" to "7 - very confident" after each prior belief question.
Next, participants are randomized into four groups: two uninformed control groups and two treatment groups. The treatment groups receive information stating that boys, on average, perform 16 points better in the mathematics section of the standardized test or that girls, on average, perform 21 points better in the reading section of the standardized test. Following the provision of this information, participants are asked to express their support or opposition to the introduction of gender-specific support to compensate for performance differences between girls and boys. Responses are collected on a five-point Likert scale ranging from "1 - strongly favor" to "5 - strongly oppose."
Control group 1:
Do you favor or oppose introducing gender-specific support to compensate for differences in mathematics performance between girls and boys?
Treatment group 1:
The PISA test has shown that boys score on average 16 points higher than girls in the mathematics test.
Do you favor or oppose introducing gender-specific support to compensate for differences in mathematics performance between girls and boys?
Control group 2:
Do you favor or oppose introducing gender-specific support to compensate for differences in reading performance between girls and boys?
Treatment group 2:
The PISA test has shown that girls score on average 21 points higher than boys in the reading test.
Do you favor or oppose introducing gender-specific support to compensate for differences in reading performance between girls and boys?
I then randomize participants from the uninformed control groups into two additional groups. One uninformed control group and one group that receives information on average performance gaps between girls and boys (boys 3 points better), of children with and without migration background (children without a migration background 29 points better), and for children from parents with higher or lower socioeconomic status (children from parents with a goof background 40 points better). Participants then state whether they perceive the disparity to be a problem on a four point Likert scale (from “1 - not a problem at all", to “4 - a very serious problem").
Control group:
What do you think, are unequal opportunities for children from the following groups in the German education system a serious problem?
Unequal opportunities for
i) … children from good or difficult social backgrounds.
ii) … boys and girls.
iii) … children with and without a migration background.
Treatment group:
What do you think, are unequal opportunities for children from the following groups in the German education system a serious problem?
Unequal opportunities for
i) … children from good or difficult social backgrounds (those from good backgrounds score 40 points better).
ii) … boys and girls (boys are 3 points better).
iii) … children with and without a migration background (those without a migration background are 29 points better).
In an obfuscated follow-up, I also investigate treatment effects on the perception of public spending in education. Participants take the follow-up survey about 14 days after the initial survey. In the follow-up survey, I ask participants:
i) What do you guess, how much is spent on average each year per student on public general schools in Germany? [Answer as an integer in Euros.]
ii) In your opinion, should public spending for schools in Germany increase, decrease, or stay the same? [Answer on a five point Likert scale from “1 - greatly increase", to "5 - greatly decrease".]
References:
Vera Freundl, Elisabeth Grewenig, Franziska Kugler, Philipp Lergetporer, Ruth Schüler, Katharina Werner, Katharina Wedel, Olivia Wirth, Ludger Wößmann, „The ifo Education Survey 2014–2021: A New Dataset on Public Preferences for Education Policy in Germany“, Jahrbücher für Nationalökonomie und Statistik, 2022.
Haaland, I., Roth, C., & Wohlfart, J. (2023). Designing information provision experiments. Journal of Economic Literature, 61(1), 3-40.